Wednesday, May 5, 2010

Women’s Education and Political Participation


It seems evident that formal education should be strongly associated with political participation for women and for men. Indeed, the American sociologists Burns, Schlozman and Verba assert, on the basis of decades of research into the factors influencing women and men’s engagement with politics in the USA, that education is an ‘especially powerful predictor of political participation’ (2001:286). They identify a range of direct and indirect effects that formal education has upon political participation. Its direct effects include the acquisition of the knowledge and communication skills useful for public debate, and direct training in political analysis through courses with current events content. Its indirect effects are many and include the benefits of voluntary engagement in school government, clubs, sports, and school newspapers; these arenas provide young people with an early apprenticeship for politics, where they can exercise leadership, develop civic skills of cooperation and negotiation, and acquire bureaucratic and organizational skills useful for political activity. Education enhances other factors supporting political engagement, such as access to high-income jobs that provide the resources and contacts for political activity, and access to non-political associations such as charitable organizations or religious establishments that can be a recruitment ground for political activity (ibid: 141-2).

There is a wide variation between countries, however, in the relationship between women’s education levels and their representation in formal politics, and their participation in other political activity. The United States, which outranks other industrialized democracies in terms of the numbers of women in higher education (and in the work force, and in professional positions), has seen persistently low numbers of women in formal politics, reaching an all-time high of just 14.3% of Congress in 2002 (Center for Voting and Democracy, 2003). Uganda, Rwanda, and Mozambique, among the poorest countries in the world with female adult literacy levels of just 41, 60.2 and 28.7 percent respectively, have parliaments in which between 25 to 30 percent of legislators are women. This contrast suggests that the connection between education and engagement in formal representative politics is not directly observable, and invites us to explore the nature of the relationship between women’s education and political participation. This paper begins by distinguishing between political representation and political participation more broadly. It assesses the importance of education among the many factors commonly associated with individual and group political engagement and effectiveness. This is done by reviewing recent statistical analyses of the relationship between these factors and variations in women’s participation in formal politics around the world. The paper ends with a consideration of the role of women’s education in advancing their interests at the level of local government, which has seen rising numbers of women participants in countries around the world.



Political Participation

Political participation matters a great deal for women as a group and as individuals. Whether women work together to protest gender-based injustices or whether they participate in non-gender-specific associations and struggles, the most important group benefit from political participation is influence on decision-making to make public policies sensitive to the needs of the group in question. For groups, participation also builds social trust and capital, and provides a form of democratic apprenticeship; it offers socialization in the norms of reciprocity and cooperation, the capacity to gain broader perspectives on particular problems in order to develop a sense of the common good. For individuals, political participation builds civic skills, while successful lobbying can result in improvements in personal welfare and status. Explanations for the very slow progress women have made in gaining political office around the world have been multi-causal, including: their lack of time for politics due to their domestic obligations, their lack of socialization for politics, their lower social capital and weaker asset base than men owing to discrimination in schools and in the market, their underrepresentaiton in the jobs that favor political careers, their marginalization within male-dominated parties, their inability to overcome male and incumbent bias in certain types of electoral systems (Randall, 1987; Matland and Taylor, 1997; Rule, 1981).

Women’s political participation is most often measured in terms of the numbers of women to be found in formal politics, in positions of public office to which they have been elected. This extremely crude measure is made even more so by the tendency to limit it to the numbers of women in the main legislative house at the national level, excluding not just numbers of women in regional and local government, but numbers of women elected as magistrates, members of the boards of public bodies such as schools or health facilities, and the like. The reasons for using this measure have to do with simple convenience. There are significant data gaps on numbers of women in local governments and other sub-national elected bodies around the world, and there is such wide variation in governance systems for sub-national communities and public bodies that they are barely comparable.

Numbers of women in representative politics are not the best indicator of the extent and intensity of women’s political participation because there is no necessary relationship between the two. Relatively large numbers of women were found in politics in socialist countries in periods when women’s independent civil society activity was suppressed under single-party governments (Molyneux, 1994). Relatively large numbers of women are found in local governments in some countries in spite of the fact that the women’s movement can be weak at these levels – for instance in France or Uganda. And India and the USA, with the largest women’s movements in the world (in terms of the sheer number and variety of women’s organizations) have some of the lowest levels of women in national office. Numbers of women active in women’s organizations, or at least numbers of women’s organizations in a country, might be a better indicator of levels of women’s political participation. Data on the strength of the women’s movement in a number of countries has been compiled by Ramirez, Soysal, and Shanahan (1997). Their indicator shows the number of women’s organizations at the national level in 1990 whose mission is expressed by themes of emancipation, political participation, democracy, or socialism. However, organizations of this type may be urban-biased and elitist and unrepresentative of the interests of the majority of women, and the existence of a large number of such organizations in a country may indicate not strength but fragmentation and therefore weakness in the women’s movement. Nevertheless it is one of the few measures available of women’s activism. This variable has not, to my knowledge, been correlated with different measures of women’s educational achievement (adult literacy, numbers of women's college and secondary school graduates, enrollment ratios). It would be a relatively simple statistical exercise to do this and worthwhile for the purpose of this EFA report.

Women’s political participation is best understood more broadly than numbers of women in office, and indeed, more broadly than numbers of women’s organizations, as women may express their political interests through participation in a wide variety of political and civic associations. Verba, Schlozman and Brady define political participation as ‘activity that has the intent or effect of influencing public action, either directly, by influencing the making of public policy, or indirectly, by influencing the selection of political decision makers’ (1995:38). This definition includes voting, campaigning for a party or supporting party work through other means (e.g. policy development, membership drives), contacting policy-makers directly by writing or telephone, protest activities, getting involved in organizations that take a stand in politics, taking part in informal efforts to solve community problems, and serving in a voluntary capacity on local governing boards such as school or zoning boards. This definition is clearly culturally-specific; notions of citizen lobbying of representatives or participating in political campaigns apply best in democratic contexts that lack violence and corruption in political competition (particularly in electoral campaigns), and that have disciplined parties with internal democracy, clear programmes and positions. This narrow definition has also been criticized by feminist political scientists as being overly focused on individual political acts and for excluding the forms of public engagement favored by women. Most insist that women’s civil society activity – what Verba et al would call non-political activity – be included in the definition of political participation (Molyneux, 2001; Marshall, 2002; Tripp 2000). Others argue that the definition of ‘political acts’ must include resistance to injustice in the private sphere, for instance in the family or the firm.
The difficulty with overly broad definitions of political participation is that activities are hard to measure, particularly acts of resistance in the private sphere, and cross-nationally comparable data are simply not available. Burns et al developed an eight-point scale of political participation that measures acts that range from voting to protest, voluntary activities in parties, religious associations, and communities (2001). This has been useful for describing differences in levels of political engagement between women and men in the USA, and for explaining its causes, but has not been tested elsewhere. They find that while women are as or more likely as men to vote, they are significantly less likely to engage in a range of other political acts, such as contacting their representatives, contributing in cash or time to campaigns, or joining a political organization (2001:2).

It is worth paying some attention to the findings of the Burns et al late 20th-century survey of factors influencing the political activity of 15,000 American adults as this is one of the most sensitive studies available. They tested the influence of the following factors on the propensity of women and men to participate in political activity: a childhood socialization in politics, educational levels, participation in high-school clubs, employment in jobs providing political connections and opportunities (e.g. professions, or unionized work), participation in non-political organizations, participation in religious organizations, available time, family income, own income, and experiences of gender-based discrimination. They found that men’s advantage in political participation was linked to a much stronger endowment of two key factors: education, and the types of jobs that provide the resources and contacts needed for politics (252). Contrary to popular expectations that time constraints and a lack of resources inhibit women’s political engagement, they found that leisure time did not differ between women and men, and that it was not related to political participation, and that while family income did have a significant impact on political activity, levels of family income differed little between women and men. Women were found to have higher endowments than men of some factors positively related to participation: participation in high school clubs and in religious associations. Interestingly, women’s experience of gender-based discrimination also produced political activity, though the study failed to measure how far this discrimination simultaneously eroded other participatory resources (259). However, women’s endowments of these participatory factors were outstripped by men’s educational and employment advantages. Also, women’s religious affiliations have ambiguous implications for their subsequent recruitment into political activity, given that some religious institutions keep women out of leadership positions.

The key finding in Burns et al is that ‘gender differences in participation are the result of disparities in the stockpile of factors that facilitate participation, not of gender differences in the way participatory factors are converted into activity’ (2001:259). This reinforces what, according to the political philosopher Anne Phillips, ‘everyone knows to be the case: that the extent to which individuals become involved in politics and thereby gain access to decision-making channels is directly correlated with the resources they have at their command; that all else being equal, those who have everything else get political power as well’ (1991:79).

This common-sense explanation of men’s advantage in political engagement goes a long way in explaining low levels of women’s political participation. However, Burns et al do not consider women’s participation in formal political institutions – in representative politics. Their study does not, therefore, help to explain why it is that even when women’s educational levels approach parity with those of men, formal political institutions remain relatively closed to women. Simply put: women’s participation in formal politics does not appear to increase in step with advances in their educational status in comparison with men. Women’s educational attainments in the USA now equal those of men, yet the persistently low numbers of women in representative positions – below the already low global average – suggest that there may be something specific to political institutions that discourages female participation. The observation about a lack of relationship between women’s education and their achievements in formal politics holds for other countries too: Jayaweera’s study of 23 middle and low income countries in Asia shows little significant difference in the level so of women in formal politics regardless of whether there is near universal education, such as in the Republic of Korea, whether there is quite extensive female educational participation (Sri Lanka), or whether there is extremely low female literacy (Pakistan and Nepal) (1997: 421). In other words, the absence of a strong linear relationship between women’s educational attainments and their numbers in formal politics suggests that there must be something specific to political institutions that discourages female participation. Jayaweera suggests that it is no great mystery: women’s many time constraints from their domestic and other work commitments, and ‘gendered perceptions of political and community leadership’ mean that women both elect to stay out of politics, and are not considered admissible as representatives even if they do wish to participate. We shall return to this problem shortly. Thus the suggestion in Burns et al that women can, as easily as men, convert endowments in ‘participatory factors’ into participatory activity, is contradicted by qualitative research into the persistence of gender-based selection and treatment biases in important non-political and political institutions, even in as deeply established a democracy as the USA (Kenworthy and Malami, 1999; Keiser et al, 2002).

In assessing the findings in Burns et al, allowance must be made for the fact that gender disparities in education are greater in older age groups in the USA – those above 40 and most likely to hold formal representative positions – than in the generation under 30 currently enjoying parity in educational achievements. Therefore we may anticipate that women’s political participation will increase as the better-educated younger generation of women mature. Indeed, because the Burns et al sample includes a substantial proportion of over-40s (mirroring their proportion of the population), it overstates the current gender gap in educational achievements, but does accurately reflect the way a residual gendered educational gap affects the political participation of the over-40s. Whether a measurable time delay exists between increases in women’s educational status and their participation in formal politics is nor clear from existing studies, and this would be a worthwhile question for the EFA team to test, using available statistics.

While the general pattern of gender-differences in participation observed by Burns et al may well hold for many other nations, there are no cross-national studies of gender-based variations in the kinds of political activities they measure, mainly because of a lack of consistent data on gender differences in voting behavior, protest activity, voluntary community activity and so on. Explorations and explanations of gender gaps in political activity in other cultures must be sensitive to differing opportunities available for political participation given variations in political institutions and cultures.
Given the difficulties of measuring the quantity and nature of women’s political participation cross-nationally, we fall back upon the number of women in office, currently the only consistent and comparable source of data showing variations in women’s engagement in politics. Though far from an ideal indicator of levels of women’s political engagement, it is not entirely unrelated to the question of women’s relative political effectiveness in any particular country. The presence of more than average (currently the global average is about 15% of lower houses - IPU 2003) numbers of women in politics should indicate that some of the many obstacles to women’s political participation have been overcome. Overcoming any of these obstacles is to some extent contingent upon the success of the women’s movement or other civil and political associations in challenging the biases that differently select women and men into social, economic, and political institutions, and produce unequal and unjust treatment of women once they do gain access. Therefore the number of women in office must at least in part reflect the strength and achievements of women’s political activism. Attentiveness to the numbers of women in office is also not irrelevant to the project of ensuring that participation in the public arena to advance women’s interests. Though women in office are almost always social and political elites lacking connections to the women’s movement there is evidence from around the world that women legislators, even when in an acute minority, help to steer political debate in parties and legislatures to issues of significance to women and children (Lijphart, 1991; Rule and Hill, 1996; McDonagh, 2002; Thomas, 1994; Vega and Firestone, 1995; Chattopadhyay and Duflo, 2001).


Gender and Political Office: Determinants of Selection

A number of cross-national comparative studies of the determinants of variations in numbers of women in office have been produced since the early 1980s. All of these seek to establish the relative contribution of political factors (the design of the electoral system, the partisan composition of government), socio-economic factors (women’s educational levels, labor market strength), demographic factors (population size, levels of urbanization) and cultural factors (dominant religion) in determining the proportion of legislative seats held by women. Data on these variables are most consistent for a sample of 15 to 25 most developed democracies, and analyses of these have found political factors, particularly the distinction between Proportional Representation (PR) and majority-plurality electoral systems to be the strongest predictor of women’s electoral success, followed by educational attainments, labor force participation, and cultural factors, such as the divide between Catholicism and Protestantism (Norris 1985, 1987; Darcy, Welch, and Clark, 1994; Lovenduski and Norris 1993). Leaving out the design of electoral institutions, Rule found that 70% of the variation in the numbers of women in politics in 19 OECD democracies between 1987 and 1991 was explained by a combination of the number of women college graduates, women in the workforce, the length of women’s suffrage, and the level of unemployment (Rule 1994:20).

Applied to a broader set of countries that includes developing nations, these types of studies have had less consistent findings. Paxton’s analysis of 108 countries found effects for political and cultural factors but not for socioeconomic variables. Two other studies are more ambiguous, finding stronger results for socio-economic and cultural factors than for political variables (Oakes and Almquist, 1993; Matland 1998). Matland, for instance, compared a sample of advanced industrialized democracies with a sample of developing country democracies. He found that whereas a shift from a majoritarian to a PR system in an industrialized democracy would produce a 15.6% jump in the female proportion of the national legislature, this electoral system variable had a coefficient ten times smaller for developing countries (1998:115 – 117). The percentage of adult women with a university education in industrialized democracies had no statistically significant effect on the numbers of women in legislatures. Data on women’s higher educational attainment was not used for his developing country sample, and instead a composite measure of women’s cultural standing in relation to men was used. This brought together three measures: the ratio of women’s to men’s literacy, the ratio of women’s labor force participation to men’s, and the ratio of university educated women to men educated in universities. Matland found, in a regression analysis of 16 less developed countries, that this measure of women’s comparative standing in relation to men explained more variance in women’s share of legislative seats than did the electoral system variable (women’s comparative cultural status explained 2.5% of variations in numbers of legislative seats, while electoral systems explained just 1.56%). However, he found that this finding was not robust. The cultural variable had been used in tandem with a measure of development, and he found that the two had split the variances between them with all the positive factors loading on the cultural variable and the negative factors onto the development variable. When he dropped the development variable, the cultural variable plummeted to less than half its previous size and was no longer significant (ibid: 118). He had to conclude that women’s representation in LDCs ‘may largely be determined by idiosyncratic conditions within that country and not by broad forces influencing all LDCs’ (ibid: 118). Oakes and Almquist’s regression analysis for 93 countries found women’s labor force participation to be significant, accounting for the greatest amount of variation in women’s legislative positions (1993:76). Interestingly, they found no positive effect for the proportion of women in managerial positions, arguing that ‘women’s skills and experiences in positions of one sector of authority are not necessarily [felt] in other sectors’ (ibid 78).

These studies were conducted on data from the 1980s, and did not capture the effect of the global wave of democratization in the late 1980s and early 1990s. Two studies that use data from the 1990s are Reynolds’ comparison of factors determining the access of African women to legislatures with factors explaining numbers of women in politics globally (in a sample of 180 countries) (1999), and Kenworthy and Malami’s study of 146 countries (1999). These studies broadly distinguish between political variables, socio-economic variables, and cultural variables in clustering factors held to determine proportions of women in politics. Though they differ in the variations they chose to highlight between political systems, and in the proxies used to measure culture, both find electoral systems to be the strongest predictor of women’s political recruitment, with variants of Proportional Representation systems producing the highest numbers of women in office. Other variables found to be significant include the length of time since the extension of women’s suffrage (and hence public familiarity with the idea of women having access to political office), higher levels of political competition (the number of parties), and the presence of strong left-of-centre parties. They find religion, as a proxy for culture, to have a significant effect on women’s representation.

source:portal.unesco.org

Outsourcing the Women’s Suffrage


Pakistan: Even Pakistan which has been struggling to have a working democracy has fared better. Benazir Bhutto was the Prime Minister of Pakistan from 1998-1990 and again from 1993-1996. Be reminded, if corruption is a requirement, Benazir probably (can’t say for sure since no concrete evidence was presented) fared just as well as her male counterparts. She was ousted on charges of corruption.

Bangladesh: Think twice before you ridicule the supposedly draconian societies found in some of the Islamic nations. Bangladesh might not be able to match the US in GDP but on this front they win hands down; Sheikh Hasina Wajed was the Prime Minister of Bangladesh from 1996-2001.

So there you have it take your pick, and learn your lesson. Considering the best America has been able to do with more than 200yrs of independence (Hillary Clinton); in my opinion the only way forward is to have a special provision (like the recent nuclear deal) for India, there by allowing an Indian woman to be the President of USA. Case in point, Indra Nooyi CEO Pepsi Co.


source:www.zuberon.com

WOMEN SUFRAGE IN PAKISTAN


So many jokes about women's suffrage: What about this: "Fear keeps women from voting booths in Pakistan?"
I keep seeing these jokes about women's suffrage and questions in GWS like "do women still need suffrage", when women in Pakistan are too afraid to vote because of suicide bombing threats from militants. Pakistani candidates were warned not to bring their female supporters to ballot booths.

Is "harmless joking" used to minimize what women actually experience?

This is from the article "Fear keeps women from voting booths in Pakistan":
As Ms. Zahid, the zoology professor, packed up her polling station on Monday night she said she was filled with a sense of relief and despair. Only 280 of the 2,058 women registered to vote in her district had cast ballots. She said she was frustrated by the low turnout but relieved that women had stayed home — and alive. “In a democratic society, everyone should vote,” she said. “But in this situation, life is more important than voting.”

source: answers.yahoo.com